source file: m1454.txt Date: Mon, 22 Jun 1998 00:12:00 EDT Subject: Appreciate your thoughts Graham From: As we discuss impacts of different tunings on different instruments, my= =0Aappreciation for the very great interrelationship between "tuning" and= =0A"timbre" deepens. Doing a bit of "free association", I recall working= with a=0Agraduate student working with synthesized flute timbres based o= n analysis of=0Arecorded flute notes played into a microphone (mono) and = converted to 16 bit=0Asigned integer samples through a A/D converter at a= 40 KHz sampling rate. Although for the particular note - a D4 - we were working with the fifth= =0Apartial was very prominent, we didn't hear it distinctly as a tone in = its own=0Aright within the overall sound of the flute D4. However, when = I artificially=0Astrengthened that partial individually among the other p= artials forming the=0Atotal sound, leaving the other partials as they had= been (based on additive=0Aresynthesis of an analyzed flute note), we cou= ld hear that partial's F#6 sound=0Aseparate from the overall sound. Then= , if we listened for it, we could=0Awithout difficulty hear the 5th parti= al's F#6 tone individually within the=0Aoriginal accurately resynthesized= flute note and I believe within the=0Arecording of the original flute no= te (I'm not sure we tested that, but in any=0Acase those two sounds were = very close - nearly indistinguishable and that=0Abeing the case it would = seem unlikely that if the fifth partial could be=0Adistinguished in the a= dditively resynthesized sound, it would be=0Aindistinguishable within the= original recorded sound). Thus when we listen to a sound, depending on the mind set with which we l= isten=0Ato it, we may hear - very clearly and fully - a "whole" or "gesta= lt" sound, or=0Awe may hear a "composite" sound within which, along side = the "gestalt", we=0Aperceive individual "component" sounds. I guess this= isn't utterly different=0Ain essence from listening to a chord sung or p= layed on a piano or other=0Amusical instrument - we easily hear the indiv= idual notes of the chord, while=0Aat the same time we experience an effec= t of the chord also as, in a way, a=0Asingle "gestalt". However in the c= ase of the chord, we tend to be much more=0Aaware of the individual compo= nents of the chord and don't habitually think of=0Athe chord as a single = entity in the same way we would think of a flute note=0Awithin which we c= an, it we try to do it, distinguish individual component=0Atones and hear= them as distinct from the overall sound of the note, which we=0Anormally= hear as a single entity. Another example of this is the case of listening to a piano note, note pa= ir,=0Aetc. There's a lot of beating going on - even if one stops all but= one single=0Astring of a unison bank and the damper pedal is up so that = all the other=0Astrings of the piano are mostly damped, frequently one ca= n hear "false beats"=0Ain that single string. I'm not a piano tuner - I = admire them and may become=0Aone - and when my piano tuner hits note pair= s and tells me he is listening to=0Awhat, say, the fourth partial is doin= g, I feel a sense of frustration because=0AI am not hearing perceptively = what he is hearing. (Based on my experience=0Awith other things, I belie= ve that if I made an effort - really applied myself=0Ato it - I could lea= rn to hear the things he's listening for). The point is=0Athat the "soun= d behind the sound" of a piano note, note pair, chord, etc. is a=0Acompos= ite of a great many sounds which might be distinguished within the=0Aover= all sound if one were to focus one's attention in such a way as to=0Adist= inguish a particular one of the many component sounds. What this leads me to is an explanation for the fact that a particular tu= ning=0Amay sound reasonably good on one musical instrument while it may s= ound=0Aterrible on another musical instrument. I'm thinking of my experi= ence with=0Atwo pianos - an older one which I purchased second hand with = a warm, not very=0Abright tone quality, on which I've found sixth comma M= T temperament to sound=0Avery beautiful, if not harmonically clear, and a= more "iron and steel" kind of=0AYamaha upright which I'm renting which h= as a very bright tone quality with a=0Alot of sound in the high frequency= region (I'd guess from 3 KHz up to 10 KHz=0Aor beyond), on which sixth c= omma MT sounds harsh and strained and=0Adisappointing, although EQT sound= s even harsher and "just awful" on that=0Apiano, which nevertheless has a= very beautiful, pleasant, gentle sound in=0Aquarter comma MT. The effec= t is so striking that one finds it hard to resist=0Abelieving that the Ya= maha engineers did a lot of careful testing of their=0Apiano and designed= it from the ground up as an instrument meant to be tuned to=0Aquarter co= mma MT, never intending that it should be misused by tuning it to=0AEQT. = One could go on speculating that in their inner deliberations, the=0Aeng= ineers and management had come to the conclusion that EQT would be around= =0Afor such a short while longer that they could safely leave it out of t= heir=0Acalculations :) It would seem that a tuning such as sixth comma MT, while it will pull th= e=0Abeat frequencies down below the dissonance "threshold" in lower parts= of the=0Aaudio spectrum so that an instrument without an abundance of hi= gh harmonics,=0Abut with quite a lot of sound lower more in the middle of= the audio spectrum,=0Amight benefit greatly as a result of its tuning be= ing changed from EQT to=0Asixth comma MT, will, with a very bright instru= ment having a lot of its sound=0Ain the high frequency region, leave the = dissonance beat frequencies in that=0Ahigh frequency part of the audio sp= ectrum still high enough to be in the Plomp=0Adissonance region for beati= ng frequencies so that the piano still sounds=0Ashrill and harsh even in = sixth comma MT. There are quite a few considerations which suggest this explanation. The= re is=0Aa quotation from around 1876 (I don't recall the source) to the e= ffect that it=0A"takes a really modern piano to show up how bad EQT sound= s". If the earlier=0Ainstruments were much less bright than the later on= es, it could be that, even=0Ain EQT tuning, they produced much less sound= in frequency ranges where the=0Adissonance beat rates were fast enough t= o be in the unpleasant range - which=0Aalso could help to explain why EQT= gained such widespread acceptance. We=0Ausually think of the guitar, pa= rticularly a gut or nylon stringed one, as a=0Apleasingly melodious music= al instrument, although until very recently, they=0Ahave been normally tu= ned to EQT. If the harmonies are not the clearest and=0Apurest, still th= e instrument has a very pleasing sound. Such nylon stringed=0Aguitars ha= ve a mild not=96bright timbre and the dissonance beatings of EQT seem=0At= o be below the dissonance threshold in the lower frequency ranges where m= ost=0Aof the audio spectral energy of the guitar comes from. In the case of the organ, however, particularly one with a bright timbre,= the=0Adissonance beating of EQT will be very prominent. William Pole re= marked on=0Athe change which retuning from a mean tone temperament (Would= this likely have=0Abeen fifth comma MT?): "The modern practice of tuning= all organs to equal=0Atemperament has been a fearful detriment to their = QUALITY OF TONE. Under the=0Aold tuning an organ made harmonious and att= ractive music, which it was a=0Apleasure to listen to, even though it mig= ht be interrupted by a "wolf" now and=0Athen. Now, the harsh thirds, app= lied to the whole instrument=0Aindiscriminately, give it a cacophonous an= d repulsive effect, which people are=0Aglad enough to run away from." (f= rom his Philosophy of Music 1879 edition, p.=0A153) Earlier he had writt= en (regarding the equal tempered scale) "...The most=0Aimportant errors a= re those of the (ital.) THIRDS, which are considerably=0Awrong; and it is= undoubtedly the fact that the major third is an interval in=0Aregard to = which, from its prominent place in the major triad (the chord of=0Anature= ), the ear is peculiarly sensitive. ON THE PIANOFORTE THIS IS NOT OF SO= =0AMUCH CONSEQUENCE; but in the organ and harmonium, where the tones are= =0Asustained, a moderately sensitive ear finds the equally=96tempered maj= or third=0Avery harsh and disagreeable." Pole finds that EQT doesn't have a seriously detrimental effect on the to= ne=0Aquality of the piano, or he makes this concession re the piano in or= der to=0Agive the impression that he is taking a fair, balanced approach = in the face of=0Acriticism which he likely anticipates from advocates of = EQT, who by 1879 in=0AEngland were increasingly coming to enjoy the taste= of victory in their=0Aefforts to see the elegant, nifty 12 TET scale est= ablished as the single=0Auniversal basis for music. He attributes this t= o the fact that the piano=0Anotes decay away fairly rapidly. It seems to= me that the pianos which he was=0Aused to hearing also had timbres which= were much less bright than those of=0Asome of today's pianos. I'd like to mention that I can count off at least ten people to whom I've= =0Apresented contrasting examples of music played on the Yamaha upright i= n EQT=0Aand quarter comma MT tuning who have very clearly and definitely = indicated to=0Ame that they found the piano to sound "warmer" in quarter = comma MT, to "sing"=0Ain that tuning. Some have said that the piano soun= ds like a fine expensive=0Ainstrument in quarter comma MT while it sounds= like a cheap piano when in EQT=0Atuning. One person writing anonymously= in a listening research test which I=0Agave wrote: "I appreciate the sou= nd of mean tone tuning; it's just different=0Aenough to be almost exotic.= I think it's just beautiful." Although not all=0Apeople have agreed wi= th me that there is a dramatic difference between the=0Asound of the pian= o in EQT and in 1/4 comma MT, a great many people have - on=0Athe order o= f half those who have heard the contrasting sounds - more than half=0Aof = those with whom I've discussed the matter personally. Getting to the matter of tuning and timbre of the piano, and of the "E=0A= pluribus unum" phenomenon. When one strikes a note, say a G4, with the p= edal=0Adown - i.e. damper off the other strings, leaving them free to vib= rate along=0Awith the struck bank of three unisons in this case, a very g= reat number of=0Aindividual tones are produced. First there are the sets= of partials of each=0Aof the three G4 unisons. Following the striking o= f these strings, perhaps=0Afrom 20 to 30 or more (a third of all the stri= ngs of the piano) strings/sets=0Aof strings are set into vibration, the t= ime courses (amplitude and to some=0Aextent - I think within a range of 2= cents or so - frequency envelopes) of=0Atheir sounds, each being compose= d of a set of partials, being individual to=0Athe particular string. Bel= ow G4, the G3 string, the C3 string, the G2 string,=0Athe Eb2 string, and= so on down will be set into vibration. This can be=0Ademonstrated by qu= ietly pressing, say, the C3 note so that its individual=0Adamper is lifte= d off the C3 string pair without these strings being set into=0Avibration= , and then striking the G4 note and releasing the key so that the G4=0Ast= rings are then stopped. One will continue to hear a reduced G4 tone unti= l=0Aone releases the C3 key. Above G4, one will be able to hear tones of= variable=0Aloudness depending on tuning when any key for a note whose fu= ndamental=0Afrequency is close to the frequency of one of the G4 partials= is depressed and=0Aheld down and then the G4 is struck and the key for G= 4 then released. Just=0Aplaying around with this roughly, I've found tha= t when the tuned frequency for=0Athe upper string lies within about 5 to = 10 cents of the frequency of the lower=0Astring's partial corresponding t= o that string, the upper string will begin to=0Avibrate. The closer its = tuned frequency to the frequency of an upper partial=0Aof the lower strin= g, the louder will be its sympathetic vibration sound. With the piano tuned to quarter comma MT temperament and with my piano tu= ner's=0Ahaving used some "octave stretching" - perhaps amounting to nearl= y 50 cents=0Aover the piano's 7 octave range - less in the piano's middle= range, more at=0Athe low and high ends, I find that there are strong sym= pathetic vibration=0Aresponses for strings at one or more octaves above t= he lower string and for=0Astrings at 2 or 3 octaves plus a third above th= e lower string (eight notes Eb,=0ABb, F, ... to E - no detectable respons= es for notes at "wolf" thirds above B,=0AF#, C#, and G#). The strings at= 1 and 2 octaves plus a fifth above a lower=0Anote vibrate sympatheticall= y but not very strongly, which seems to result from=0Atheir being tuned (= nominally) 5.4 cents flat from just. There are strong=0Aresponses for th= e C# and G# strings 2 octaves and a seventh above Eb and Bb=0Arespectivel= y when the latter are struck (nominally 3 cents flat from being=0Ajust 7/= 1 s above the lower notes). With the piano in 12 TET, I have sometimes= =0Afound weak responses for strings two octaves and an EQT (nominally 13.= 7 cents=0Asharp from just) major third above lower strings, and sometimes= these=0Aresponses have been too weak for me to detect. Well, all this opens up a "world" of things happening in the piano when e= ven=0Aone single note is struck. It occurs to me also that when strings = are excited=0Ato vibrate in sympathy with a lower string, they in their t= urn, albeit weakly,=0Awill exert some effect on the strings both above an= d below them. Even some of the physically low amplitude components of the overall sound= -=0Adown 50 or 60 dB or more - could, depending on exactly where and wha= t they=0Aare, have an appreciable effect on the piano's overall sound. T= he pattern of=0Athis "cloud" of relatively low amplitude components could= have the effect of=0Acontributing a delightful, difficult=96to=96describ= e, "feel" to the piano's=0Aoverall sound or it could fail to have such a = potentially achievable effect or=0Aeven detract from the pleasantness of = the instrument's sound. Your observations re 1/6 comma MT and 1/9 comma MT on the digital piano,= =0AGraham, help to fill in this field of data out of which we're trying t= o=0Adiscern underlying patterns. These patterns, to the extent they appl= y=0Agenerally, in turn can provide us with new, more detailed, and more p= owerful=0Aguidance towards creating really delightful music, gaining cont= rol over those=0Aelusive "performance variables" which can, as if by magi= c, if "all working=0Aright" make a performance sound great. Dave Hill La Mesa, CA